Musical Hallucinations: Causes, Conditions and Treatment
the Frost School of Music at the University of Miami
Musical Hallucinations: Causes, Conditions and Treatment
Musical hallucinations (MH) can be described as perceptions of musical sounds in the
absence of external auditory stimuli. Although imagined sounds can be non-musical; such as
bells, whistles and sirens, case studies indicate that music “[takes] precedence over all other
auditory hallucinations” (Sacks, 2006). Furthermore, MH may often take the form of songs from
childhood and may be connected with strong childhood emotions. Although the causes of MH
are not always known, they affect at least 2% of individuals affected by presbyacousis (Sacks,
2006). In an important literature review, Evers and Ellger (2004) reviewed 132 cases involving
MH and found that 61% of the case reports had a history of hearing impairment or deafness.
They also found a “female preponderance of 70% and a mean age of 61.5 years.” Although there
may be a connection with functional psychopathological conditions such as depression and
obsessive compulsive disorder (OCD), it is important to note that MH is not necessarily
indicative of psychosis (Mahendran, 2007).
Recently on National Public Radio, reporter Robert Krulwich (2008) interviewed Cheryl
C. (not her real name) regarding her experience with MH. Affected by presbyacousis, Cheryl had
lost almost all of her hearing. She recollects how one night when she was reading she heard a
terrible cacophony of sounds: the tolling of bells, rumbling trolley cars, clanging metal and
crashing cymbals to name a few. Yet when she looked out the window to see what was creating
the noise she came to the realization these noises were in her head. After approximately twenty
minutes, the noise was replaced by music, starting with the song “Michael Row Your Boat
Ashore,” then followed by hymns, spirituals, patriotic songs and other familiar songs. Fearful
that she might be “going nuts,” Cheryl C. went to visit neurologist Oliver Sacks who, after
extensive tests, explained to her “that because her hearing was so compromised…[and] hardly
any sounds were coming into her brain, the cells in her brain dedicated to hearing…were under
stimulated…[and] began making stuff up” (Krulwich, 2008).
Positron emission tomography (PET) scans involving six individuals with backgrounds
similar to Cheryl C. revealed that a network of regions in the brain became more active as her
MH became more intense (Griffiths, 2000). The activation described is typical of the pattern in
normal people who are listening to music (Zimmer, 2005). Griffiths (2000) found that the PET
scans revealed no difference between normal musical listening and MH with one exception; PET
scans of subjects experiencing MH do not indicate activation of the primary auditory cortex. This
makes sense because there would be very little, if any input from the peripheral hearing
apparatus due to hearing loss. These findings also support the phenomenon of MH in non-
hearing impaired individuals who are placed in an environment devoid of auditory stimuli.
Although manifestations typically occur in environments devoid of aural stimuli, that is not the
Satoh, Kokubo & Kuzuhara (2007) describe a case study involving a 75-year-old woman
who, in July 2002, began hearing “A red sunset,” a Japanese nursery song from a neighboring
town office near the hospital her ailing husband was admitted. The MH subsided only when she
spoke, sang, or listened to music. To stop the MH she resorted to singing various songs. These
songs that she sang, which had no apparent significance to her, became incorporated in her MH.
She reported hearing the hallucinated songs with the text and accompaniment by the “ohayashi,”
a traditional Japanese vocal accompaniment consisting of short untuned voices. Meanwhile, the
timbre of “A red sunset” had changed from a chorus of children to her own voice. In September
2002, the woman was admitted to the hospital for testing. Medical and psychological tests
revealed no abnormalities. Her hearing was unimpaired as indicated by pure tone audiometry.
She also received a normal score on the Hamilton Depression Scale as administered by her
psychiatrist who subsequently ruled out psychogenic disease. Her prescribed treatment for this
condition consisted of carbamazepine, diazepam and antidepressants for 6 months with little
improvement. Eventually the MH subsided and by September 2004, she was experiencing MH
Satoh et al. (2007) surmise that one possible explanation for this case of MH is that
accompaniment and timbre may be independent components of musical perception which are
stored separately in the brain. When the information is retrieved from memory, the components
are combined with the melodic structure of the familiar or frequently sung melody. It is
interesting to think of the brain as a musical “mixing board” that can construct and separate
different elements of music rather than store the idea of a musical sound as a finished and
complete product. This idea does not stray from the description provided by brain scans during
musical processing which seem to indicate several areas of brain activation in both normal
subjects and those affected by MH (Griffiths, 2000). This idea supports the concept of
modularity of musical processing and explains how manifestations of MH can still occur in
In a literature review by Evers and Ellger (2004), manifestations of MH can also be
attributed to: a) psychiatric disorder, b) brain lesion, c) epilepsy and d) intoxication. Of great
interest to researchers are individuals who experience MH with focal lesions and epileptic brain
activity. Until recently, neurologists believed in a hemispheric dominance theory of music.
Brain scans of subjects with lesions seem to refute this notion. Evers and Ellger (2004) found no
significant difference in the hemisphere associated with the legion as the major cause of MH.
Furthermore, electroencephalography (EEG) measurements in epileptic subjects neither refute
nor support the concept of hemispheric dominance of MH.
When MH occurs in musicians, researchers are highly motivated to study the phenomena.
A recent report cites an 83-year-old, female, retired secondary school music teacher who was
diagnosed with a probable brainstem stroke and began experiencing MH (Warren & Schott,
2006). Although pure tone audiometry indicated mild presbyacousis, there was no practical
deafness or tinnitus reported. Remarkably, none of the tunes associated with this subject’s MH
seemed familiar. She continued to derive pleasure from music, and to play the piano. On two
occasions, she notated her hallucinations which can be reduced to a series of repeating motifs,
three to five notes in length, comprised predominantly of skips of thirds. This report seems to
defy the notion that MH is dependent solely upon memory, but may also be based on musical
experience and training. It also serves as an example of how the musical brain may be different
from individuals without musical training and experience.
For individuals, the conditions associated with MH are perceived as dysfunctional.
Therefore, consultation with a neurologist typically results in some kind of treatment. In the case
of Cheryl C., she underwent a cochlear implant, hoping that stimulation of the primary auditory
cortex would relieve her of MH. In her case the procedure was not successful in that she now
experiences a rather hollow sounding impression of sound through her new implant in addition to
the high fidelity sounds of her MH (Sacks, 2007). Neurologists in the Netherlands (Gilhaus, Dara
Hama-Amin & Renier, 2007) found that treatment of MH may be the result of an ancillary
reaction to medication prescribed for epilepsy. In a case involving a healthy 70-year-old woman
who suddenly started hearing children’s songs, an MRI revealed leukoaraiosis, a reduction in the
density of cerebral white matter. EEG readings indicated spike-wave discharges on the left
temporal side of her brain. She was administered carbamazepine for her seizures which also
partially relieved the MH. Another patient, an 85-year-old woman diagnosed with leukoaraiosis
also complained of hearing children’s and religious songs from her youth. While hearing songs
an EEG revealed epileptic discharges of the right temporal lobe. The drug levetiracetam was
administered to counter her seizures which also reduced the amount and intensity of the MH
Doctors in Japan (Ukai, Yamamoto, Tanaka, Shinosaki & Takeda, 2007) describe the
case of an 82-year-old woman suffering from hearing loss in both ears. She began experiencing
MH consisting of voices and old familiar songs accompanied by musical instruments almost all
day long. Other than the hearing loss, tests revealed no brain defect. After one week of the
administration of carbamazepine, the intensity of the MH subsided, but still persisted.
Approximately two years later, the patient consented to a regimen of donezepil, a drug approved
only for the treatment of Alzheimer’s disease. After two-weeks of treatment, she reported that
her MH subsided and that she only experienced them when focusing her attention in quiet
environments. Ukai, et al. (2007) speculate that certain drugs such as donezepil may help support
sensory inputs that inhibit the generation of memory traces, in this case songs from childhood,
when the threshold of auditory stimuli is diminished.
In most cases, memory plays an important role in creating and recreating musical
experience, especially for those who experience MH. Further study of this phenomenon may
help explain why individuals experience and describe the same musical stimuli differently. This
is of particular importance to music fields that rely on critique such as adjudication, evaluating,
musical criticism in the media and education, especially in absence of criteria-specific rating
scales. The notion that musical material in MH involve memories of music from childhood not
heard in decades suggests the powerful impact early childhood music education can have on
long-term memory storage and retrieval. Whether the impact on memory is music specific or
generic is unknown. Nonetheless, the very idea that aural hallucinations tend to be music specific
as opposed to random sound is remarkable. It is as if “our auditory systems, our nervous systems,
Evers, S. & Ellger, T. (2004). The clinical spectrum of musical hallucinations. Journal of the Neurological Sciences, 227, 55-65.
Gilhuis, H. J., Dara Hama-Amin, A. & Renier, W. (2007, June 12). Songs from childhood in the
head [Letter to the editor]. Clinical Otolaryngology, 32, 419-420.
Griffiths, T. D. (2000). Musical hallucinosis in acquired deafness: Phenomenology and brain
substrate. Brain, 123, 2065-2076.
Krulwich, R. (2008, January 29). Hearing things: When sounds come unbidden. National Public Radio, Krulwich on Science. Retrieved April 16, 2008, from
http://www.npr.org/templates/story/story.php?storyId=17818400
Mahendran, R. (2007). The psychopathology of musical hallucinations. Singapore Medical
Sacks, O. (2006). The power of music. Brain, 129, 2528-2532.
Sacks, O. W. (2007). Musicophilia: tales of music and the brain. New York: Knoph.
Satoh, M., Kokubo, M. & Kuzuhara, S. (2007). A case of idiopathic musical hallucination with
increasing repertoire. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery and Psychiatry, 78, 203-204.
Ukai, S., Yamamoto, M., Tanaka, M., Shinosaki, K. & Takeda, M. (2007). Donepezil in the
treatment of musical hallucinations. Psychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences, 61, 190-192.
Warren, J. D. & Schott, G. D. (2006). Musical hallucinations in a musician. Journal of
Zimmer, C. (2005, July 12). Neuron network goes awry, and brain becomes an IPod. New York Times. Retrieved March 10, 2008 from http://www.nytimes.com
EQVALAN® GOLD, the leader in spectrum of activity Effi cacy Comparative Chart Parasite species and stages treated as per * P, STRONGID , SAFE-GUARD the labels of the commercially available , STRONGID EQVALAN (Ivermectin/Praziquantel) EQVALAN (Ivermectin) QUEST PLUS EXODUS (Pyrantel pamoate) PANACUR (Fenbendazole) EQUIMAX (Ivermectin/Praziquantel) BIMECTIN
Tamilnadu Medical Services Corporation Limited, Chennai – 600 008. Tender for the supply of Drugs & Medicines for the year 2012-2013 Tender Ref.No.001/M(P)/TNMSC/DRUG/2012, Dt.27.01.2012 Tenderer wise Item Rejection List Name of the Tenderer : Adroit Pharmaceuticals Pvt. Ltd., ------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------